This is a study of the Phi Ratio of the Earth Grid and Music. Converting Phi Earth Grid distances into musical interval.
http://home.att.net/~john.aona/beginning.html
The energy lines that go into making up the grid are 'layered' in and
through the planet. Much like layers and layers of different weaves of
fabric. Each layer down - from the basic skeleton outlined here - is
exactly one third of the spacing density of the layer above it. This
continues down to the 'atomic' levels itself. Thus if you start from
some given 'zero' point, grid longitude and latitude wise, as being
common to all layers, you will get an alternating match up of grid
'fabric' lines.
PART 2 - GOLDEN SECTION SITES - ANGKOR, THE GREAT PYRAMID & NAZCA
Angkor Wat is 4,745 miles from the Great Pyramid and the Great Pyramid is 7,677 miles from Nazca. This is a precise expression of &phi, the Golden Section:
4,745 x 1.618 = 7,677
Ninety miles northeast of Angkor Wat are the Angkor temples at Prassat Preah Vihear. Prassat Vihear is 4754 miles from the Great Pyramid. The line of ancient sites crosses over the Great Pyramid and Angkor Vihear.
Twenty five miles northwest of the city of Nazca is a figure known as
the Hummingbird. The Hummingbird is 7,692 miles from the Great Pyramid.
The line of ancient sites also crosses over the Hummingbird.
The relationship between the distances from Angkor Vihear to the Great Pyramid and from the Great Pyramid to the Nazcan Hummingbird is also a precise expression of phi:
4,754 x 1.618 = 7,692
Because the distance from the Hummingbird to Angkor Vihear is one-half of the circumference of the earth, two Golden Section relationships
between these sites are shown by the circumference of the earth along
the line of ancient sites:
Angkor: Prassat Preah Vihear, Giza, Nazca. (Circle Diagram).
The Hummingbird. (jpg).
These Golden Section relationships may also be diagramed on a straight
line: (Linear Diagram).
The line of ancient sites is a line, from the perspective of the
illustration in Part One, and it is a circle, from the perspective of
the illustration in Part Six. The line and the circle are found in the
greek letter phi and the number 10. Zero and one are also the first two
numbers and the only two numbers in the binary code.
The phi relationships between these sites are reflected repeatedly in
the first 500 Fibonacci numbers. The first three prime numbers, 2, 3 and
5, approximate the intervals along the circumference of 20%, 30% and
50%, between these three sites. This same percentage of the
circumference relationship, accurate to three digits, is found in
Fibonacci numbers 137-139:
Percentage of circumference / First three digits of Fibonacci numbers:
Angkor to Giza: 19.1%...........................#137: 191...
The next prime Fibonacci number after #137 is #359. The distances
between these sites, in miles, is reflected by Fibocacci numbers
359-361, accurately to five digits:
Distance between sites / First five digits of Fibonacci numbers:
Angkor to Giza: 4,754 miles.................#359: 47542...
Jalison
On Rabbits, Mathematics and Musical Scales
Contact John S. Allen by e-mail- Bikexprt.com
John S. Allen:
"...vast numbers of tribes and cultures...developed music independently,
and in the most varied surroundings...They exhibit enormous differences
in their language, customs, clothes, modes of life and so forth, but all
who have advanced beyond homophonic music have, if not precisely the
same musical scale, at least scales which are built on the same
principle.
"The main differences are found in the numbers of notes which form the scale. By stopping at differing places in the sequence. F-C-G-D-A-...,*
we obtain the various scales which have figured in the musics of
practically all those races which have advanced beyond the one-part
music of primitive man."
* There is no theoretical reason for starting with F rather than with
any other note of the scale. F has been selected merely in order to keep
off the black notes of the piano for as long as possible.
Sir James Jeans, Science and Music, pp. 163-164
No, not the Bunny Hop, or Here Comes Peter Cottontail -- or Jefferson
Airplane's White Rabbit, or I'm Late, I'm Late (for a very important
date) from the Disney movie, or del Tredeci's Final Alice (hmmm, that
rabbit gets around...).
We're going to discuss rabbits as they relate to musical scales.
In his Liber Abaci, the 13th Century Italian mathematician Fibonacci set out to determine how fast rabbits would multiply, given certain
mathematical assumptions: a rabbit couple could reproduce once a month
beginning at the age of two months, bearing a litter of one male and one
female, and no rabbits died. Let's say that you start with one pair of
baby rabbits in January. Then, from month to month, the number of rabbit
couples will be:
Table. Fibonacci Progression.
For any month's total number of rabbits, you add the number of rabbits
existing one month earlier to the number of rabbits born since then --
the same as the number of rabbits existing two months earlier.
Generalizing this to a mathematical formula, we can say that
xn = xn-1 + xn-2 (x0 = 1, x1 = 1).
Where
x represents the number of rabbit couples,
n represents the number of the month, 0, 1, 2 etc., and so,
xn is the number of rabbits existing in month n.
But what does this rabbit formula have to do with music? Lots! If we
only substitute x0= 2 and x1 = 5 for the first two terms, we get the
series 2, 5, 7, 12, 19, 31, 50... which bears an important relationship
to the structure of musical scales. Any musician will recognize 5, 7 and
12 as the numbers of pitches per octave in pentatonic, diatonic and chr
omatic scales; and also as the number of black keys, white keys and keys
per octave of the traditional musical keyboard.
Now let's see how these numbers build musical structures. The simplest musical interval is the octave, a 2/1 frequency ratio. The next simplest are the fifth, a 3/2 ratio (more or less, in various tunings) and its inversion, the fourth (4/3, more or less). The numbers in the musical
Fibonacci series 2, 5, 7, 12, 19 ... all are generated by increasingly
long series of musical fourths and fifths, as Sir James Jeans described
and shown in the table below.
Table.
Our first division of the octave (scale no. 1 in the table) is into a
scale of two tones a fifth apart, say F and C, with a frequency ratio of
3/2. The next fifth after our two tones is a G, and the resulting
sequence of two fifths, 3/2 x 3/2, takes us to 9/4 (2.25), which brings
us close to an F a musical octave (2/1) higher than our first F. We
think of the G as the same as the F, and stop with a two-tone scale.
With two tones per octave, it is already possible to play music which
distinguishes a tonic and dominant -- like a typical tympani part of the
early Classical era.
We may also lower the fifths to produce a 2-tone equal temperament. In
fact, we can play this temperament on any conventional musical
instrument by lowering the C of our two-tone scale by a half tone to B.
But assigning different roles such as "tonic" and "dominant" to each of
the two pitches of our scale is difficult. The intervals between them
are equal, and so we can't tell which pitch has which role.
Now if we prolong the series of fifths, F-C-G-D-A-(E), the E at the end
is close to F again. If we leave the fifths mathematically exact, we can
now play pentatonic music ("Mama makes shortnin', shortnin bread ...")
in one key signature, the way we do on the black keys of any
conventional keyboard instrument. As you can see by looking at the black
keys, the pentatonic scale consists of groups of three and two pitches
separated by two larger steps. This pattern repeats in every octave,
allowing us to identify the role of the tones of a pentatonic scale.
But suppose that we raise all of the fifths a little, until the final E
is at the same pitch as F. In this way, we construct a 5-tone equal
temperament. We can't play this temperament on a regular keyboard, but
we can play it on any synthesizer which lets us set nonstandard
intervals: set the semitone or octave interval for 2.4 times the
standard one (or 1.2 times and play on alternate keys). The resulting
scale will sound somewhat out of tune to our ears which are used to
standard 12-tone equal temperament, but its melodic and harmonic
properties are clear enough. It is not only a theoretical temperament;
it is the standard Javanese tuning called slendro (see for example
Helmholtz, p. 518.)
In the 5-tone temperament, we can distinguish tonic and dominant of
two-tone scales and modulate freely among five key signatures - but
pentatonic melodies do not work. Even though there are five pitches per
octave in this scale, ordinary pentatonic music sounds oddly rootless --
we can't determine the keynote. With five equal intervals, there is no
differentiation between wide and narrow intervals to identify the
pentatonic key signature. This is the same problem we had earlier in
distinguishing the tonic and dominant in our 2-tone temperament.
Now that we have established the first two terms of our Fibonacci
series, succeeding terms follow logically. Each new, extended series of
fifths is the sum of the previous two. The reason for this not the same
as for the numbers of Fibonacci's rabbits, but is just as simple: since
sequences of two or five fifths approximate whole numbers of octaves,
sums of these numbers of fifths also approximate whole numbers of
octaves and allow us to close a circle of fifths by filling in the wider
steps in the scale which the earlier series of fifths built. So now
let's increase the number of tones per octave, just as Fibonacci did
with his rabbit population.
Continuing from 5 on to 7, the series F-C-G-D-A-E-B-(F#) once again
brings us close to F. If we leave the fifths exact, we can now play
seven-tone diatonic music (like playing on the white keys of the piano),
in one key. We can also play two-and five-tone music.
If, on the other hand, we lower all of the fifths of our seven-tone
scale a little, we arrive at a seven-tone equal temperament. Set the
semitone or octave on a synthesizer to 1.7142 (that's 12/7) times
standard to play in this temperament. In a seven-tone temperament, we
can play in the 2 and 5 tone scales and also play any ordinary
pentatonic music with full freedom of modulation in seven keys.
The seven-tone temperament is not just a theoretical construction. It is the standard tuning used in traditional Siamese music (see for example
Helmholtz, p. 556). You might think that, with 7 tones, you could also
play standard diatonic music in this temperament.
But if you try, it sounds oddly rootless. Though we have the seven
pitches needed to play diatonic music, the intervals between them are
all the same. There is no distinction between large and small intervals
to tell us what step of the musical scale we are on -- the same problem
we had playing pentatonic music in the five-tone temperament in the
previous step of our experiment, or two-tone music in the step before
that.
Next, we proceed to the series of 12 fifths,
F-C-G-D-A-E-B-F#-C#-G#-D#-A#-(E#). If we lower all of the fifths very slightly, we arrive at our familiar 12-tone equally-tempered tuning. Now we can play all of the music we played in the earlier 2, 5 and 7-tone
scales, and we can also play diatonic music using 7-tone scales with
full freedom of modulation through 12 keys.
The progression from 7 to 12 tones occurred in European music between
the Middle Ages and the Baroque era. Composers have explored the
potential of the 12-tone scale over several centuries, and these
explorations have culminated in the highly chromatic music of the later
Romantic era and "atonal" music of the 20th century.
The term "atonal" reflects the oddly rootless quality of music that
moves freely among all 12 of the tones of the scale. Does this begin to
sound familiar?
Each new temperament generated by extending the series of fifths
encompasses the melodic and harmonic possibilities of all of the earlier
ones. The mathematical logic of this progression of equal temperaments,
and the way it generates new musical possibilities at each step, lead us
to ask "what if we went on to the next step of this progression, a
19-tone temperament?"
In the 1930's, music theorist Joseph Yasser published a book, Theory of
Evolving Tonality, exploring that question. Yasser suggested that
certain composers of his time, notably Scriabin, were already composing
in a 12-tone "supradiatonic" scale with distinct tonalities identifiable
by the wide and narrow intervals a 12-tone scale would have in a 19-tone
temperament. Yasser also suggested that a logical next step in musical
instrument design would be to adopt a 19-tone temperament, which he
contended would open up possibilities in music similar to those which
followed the transition from the 7-tone to the 12-tone scale.
Why don't we use Yasser's system?
The advance to a 19-tone scale which Yasser proposed has not yet
occurred. Why? I think that there are a number of reasons.
Ingrained habits
One is that conventional musical instruments, music theory and practice
are so deeply ingrained in our musical culture that breaking away from
them would be difficult. The perception is grounded in the traditional
design of musical instruments, and particularly of keyboard instruments,
that D# and Eb (for example) are the same pitch, even though we spell
them differently in musical notation.
Keyboard difficulties
Another problem is that a practical keyboard to play in a 19-tone
temperament does not evolve simply from the traditional keyboard.
A 19-tone keyboard based on the traditional keyboard would have pairs of black keys (Db and C#, Eb and D#, etc.), and an additional black key
between the semitone intervals E-F and B-C. Every diatonic semitone
would, then, become a two-step interval, and every diatonic whole tone a
three-step interval. The octaves would have to be lengthened to make
room for the fingers, with all of the additional keys.
An alternative, favored by Yasser, would be exchange the positions of
the black and white keys, front to rear. This keyboard might be somewhat
easier to play, but it would still have long octaves and require
difficult relearning.
Poor intonation of diatonic intervals
Another fundamental reason that the 19-tone system has not developed
naturally out of the 12-tone system is that the 12-tone system works
rather well. The 2, 5, and 7-tone systems which lead up to the 12-tone
system have very impure fifths. The fifths in the 12-tone system are
quite good.
But going from 12 to 19 tones requires that we make the fifths worse
rather than better.
In 19-tone, the minor third is very pure but the major third, though
more harmonious than in the 12-tone temperament, is slightly flat,
compromising the melodic role of the leading tone.
Poor intonation of additional intervals
We should also hope that a system with more than 12 tones would provide
good approximations to additional harmonic intervals used in non-western
and experimental music (and discussed by Yasser), but the 19-tone system
does poorly at this. The 19-tone scale is, for example, not capable of
distinguishing between the 7th and 11th harmonics, or of distinguishing
the 11th from the augmented fourth.
The rule of seven plus or minus two
The seven-tone scales in the twelve-tone system approach the limit of
what the human mind can assimilate. The rule of "seven plus or minus
two" in sensory psychology states that for any sensory continuum, humans
describe between five and nine different categories: to give an example,
we describe the gray scale using the categories white, off-white, light
gray, medium gray, dark gray, near black and black. Though we can
discriminate more shades of gray when they are placed side by side for
comparison, we do not give names to them, or use the discrimination
between them as part of a conceptual structure based on unaided
observation and memory.
The musical octave is regarded by sensory psychologists as a continuum
like the gray scale, and it follows that 12 distinct pitch classes may
be more than we can easily assimilate as elements of a musical
structure.
It's not as if 19-tone wasn't tried...
Historically, the evolution toward a 19-tone scale would in fact have
succeeded if not for the problems discussed here. The mean-tone
temperament used from the late Renaissance through the early Classical
era had unequal semitones; it is possible to play Yasser's theoretical
12-tone scale in one key signature on any keyboard instrument tuned in
the mean-tone temperament. Not only this, many keyboard instruments,
especially organs, had extra keys -- for example, separate keys for Eb
and D# (see for example Jeans, p. 173). The extra keys let the
instruments play in several more key signatures than a standard 12-tone
keyboard and still avoid the discordant "wolf fifth" which resulted from
the mean tone series of fifths' not closing at 12. Handel, among others,
had an organ with such a keyboard. The logical culmination of such
keyboards with doubled sharps and flats would have been a 19-tone
system.
But unlike the transition from the 7-tone to the 12-tone keyboard, the
transition to 19 tones never occurred. Instead, 12-tone equal
temperament gradually replaced mean-tone temperament beginning around
1700 AD and culminating around 1850 AD.
(Note: It is very important to be able to ignore the talk about musical "wolves" of dissonance and the inability of the 19 note scale not fitting into what we consider "proper" interval. The 19 note scale fits with the squaring (Correction: 09/15/04 powers) of 2^18 and 2^37 (Correction: 09/15/04 2^19 and 2^38) which suggests a difference of slightly more than an octave between Leahy's 242144 and 137438953472, suggestive of the fine-structure constant.
"Then Euclid proves mathematically the impossibility of such a resolution
as this Nichomachean reasoning in Sectio Canonis, Theorem 9, (Jan 157.
5-158.7). he begins with the postulate: "Six sesquioctaval intervals are
greater than one interval in the double proportion".
"In order to set up this progression in the most economical way, Euclid
first determined the smallest rational number in the six susquioctave
intervals (or 7 terms) so that the entire series would have 8 as a
common factor",
(Correspondences, Fuller, Synergetics 905.49).
"Accordingly, 8 is squared and the resultant term 64",
(Correspondences, Fuller, Synergetics, 1106.12).
"is multiplied by 8, five more times yielding 262144 (2^18). To this term one-eighth of its total value is added, the process being reapplied to each successive result, producing thereby a series of rational number in the relationship 8 : 9. Thus, it is proven by Euclid that an octave
(2 : 1) cannot consist of six whole-tones-- as Nichomachus would have
it-- but of some fractional interval less than its total range".....)
Suppose, however, that we leap past the 19-tone temperament and examine the next step of our Fibonacci series, with 31 tones.
This temperament turns out to be extremely close to the historical
mean-tone temperament, with major thirds that are nearly pure. In
addition, it gives a nearly perfect natural seventh, and a natural 11th
which is slightly flat, but distinct from the augmented fourth.
If we put aside the idea of a 12 or 19-tone diatonic scale and regard
the 31-tone tuning primarily as a tool to improve the intonation of
traditional scales and intervals, then it looks like a sensible idea. I
have tried it on a guitar, which sounds very pleasant, although it is
difficult to play. The question of how to construct keyboard instruments
which use this temperament has been addressed by Fokker. In forthcoming
articles, I will address this tuning, among others, as they relate to
modern, computer-controlled instruments. The next article in this series
describes musical tunings as mathematical series.
© 1997 John S. Allen
© Copyright. Robert Grace. 2001
(Prime)Giza to Nazca: 30.9%......................#138: 309...
Nazca to Angkor: 50.0%..............................#139: 500...
(Prime)Giza to Nazca: 7,692 miles......#360: 76924...
Nazca to Angkor: 12,446 miles......#361: 12446...